A mysterious neurological condition makes faces look grotesque—and sheds new light on the inner workings of the brain.
Article | Archive
PMO = prosopometamorphopsia
Article | Archive
PMO = prosopometamorphopsia
In 2007, Jason Werbeloff, a twenty-two-year-old graduate student in Johannesburg, South Africa, spent months in bed with a severe case of mononucleosis. Every part of his body—his joints, his skin, his swollen throat—was in pain, and he passed the time staring at the concrete ceiling of his room. Television gave him a headache; he tried to read but often forgot the names of characters by the end of each page. He saw no one except his mother, who occasionally stopped by with groceries.
After he recovered, Werbeloff was eager to be around people again, and he spent a night clubbing. In the shifting red light, he looked at a friend’s face and realized that the right side looked odd. It seemed to stretch outward, like Silly Putty being pulled, and a dark, rough patch was visible around the friend’s right eye. Werbeloff blinked and looked away, and his friend’s features briefly returned to normal. Then the distortions appeared again. “That is when people got ugly,” Werbeloff told me.
During the Zoom call, Duchaine’s Ph.D. student Sarah Herald asked Werbeloff to stare at portrait photos for longer than he was used to. Werbeloff hadn’t realized how distended a face could become: the right side stretched until it was bulging, and the dark patch became a deep concave pit encircling the eye. After the session, Werbeloff cried. “I don’t believe in demons,” Werbeloff told me. “But I can totally understand that someone who was religious would find it a deeply religiously disturbing experience.”
Distorted perceptions are not the same as hallucinations, Blom told me. If you saw an elephant appear in your home office, you would be hallucinating. But, if you looked up and perceived an elephant in an elephantine cloud, that’s more like a distortion. “There’s a cloud—it’s actually there,” he said. He views his PMO patients as very different from psychiatric patients with schizophrenia, who hear voices or see things that don’t exist. People with PMO aren’t helped by antipsychotics; they know that what they’re seeing isn’t right.
PMO may clarify the role of each side of our brains when it sees faces. The right hemisphere seems especially important for facial perception: injury-induced face blindness tends to result from damage to the right. But PMO can apparently be caused by lesions on either side. Lesions on the left can cause distortions on the right side of people’s faces; lesions on the right can cause distortions on both sides. For this reason, Duchaine believes that the left hemisphere processes the right side of faces, and vice versa—and then, he suspects, the right side puts the pieces of the image together. “They’re fused, and they go forward together for later processing,” Duchaine said. “That’s something we didn’t know.” Researchers are able to induce PMO-like symptoms by stimulating specific parts of the brain, especially on the right side. And some hemi-PMO cases involve damage to the splenium, a part of the brain that carries information between the hemispheres.