MittEremltage
Senior Member (Voting Rights)
This is really, really weird... and definitely not okay![]()
I defenetly agree!
This is really, really weird... and definitely not okay![]()
I’m not up to speed on the roles, so I might have gotten things wrong?
Thank you so much @MittEremltage, fantastic effort, you're doing an amazing and really important job
I have tried to summarize what has happened in Sweden since the consortium's secret meeting took place in Oslo 2022.
Google translated blog post:
https://mitteremitage-wordpress-com..._sl=sv&_x_tr_tl=en&_x_tr_hl=sv&_x_tr_pto=wapp
You are doing a brilliant job of documenting all this, @MittEremltage.I have tried to summarize what has happened in Sweden since the consortium's secret meeting took place in Oslo 2022.
Google translated blog post:
https://mitteremitage-wordpress-com..._sl=sv&_x_tr_tl=en&_x_tr_hl=sv&_x_tr_pto=wapp
I should stop being shocked
A singular pseudoscientific focus can be dangerous.A singular biomedical focus can be dangerous
Their entire ideology is built on this. But of course we know that any requirement they want to apply will only apply in their favor.The Guidelines should not create artificial separation between patient groups without scientific backing (for example PEM vs non-PEM)
Responding to criticism: Modern brain research provides new insights into long-term symptoms and explains how symptoms can arise both with and without signals from the body, writes Silje Endresen Reme (pictured)
Their explanations are so banale:https://www.sciencenorway.no/covid1...is-neither-psychological-nor-physical/2503062
OPINION:
![]()
Responding to criticism: Modern brain research provides new insights into long-term symptoms and explains how symptoms can arise both with and without signals from the body, writes Silje Endresen Reme (pictured)
(Photo: Universtity of Oslo)
Long Covid is neither psychological nor physical
OPINION: We don’t believe there is one single way out of Long Covid – we believe there are several.
Silje Endresen Reme
PROFESSOR AT THE UNIVERSITY OF OSLO AND CHAIR OF THE BOARD, OSLO CHRONIC FATIGUE CONSORTIUM (OCFC)
Symptoms and the brain – a new understanding
We all recognise that patients’ symptoms – whether pain, fatigue, or nausea – are real and often disabling. Modern neuroscience provides a powerful lens for seeing how these symptoms can emerge and persist, even when no overt tissue damage or active disease process is present.
At the heart of this insight is the theory of predictive processing, a biologically grounded model of brain function. Far from merely repackaging old psychosomatic thinking, predictive processing is supported by a growing body of experimental and clinical evidence.
It describes how the brain continuously generates predictions about incoming sensory information – and how mismatches between expectation and reality can give rise to conscious sensations and, ultimately, symptoms.
Consider phantom limb pain, where an amputee still experiences 'pain' in a missing limb because the brain continues to expect signals from that area. Or the rubber hand illusion, which shows how the brain can be tricked into feeling touch or temperature in a prosthetic hand.
Even everyday experiences like conditioned nausea – where the sight or smell of food that once caused food poisoning triggers nausea weeks or months later – demonstrate how past experience shapes present sensations.
When the alarm freezes
A similar process may underlie persistent fatigue. In one study, researchers paired a neutral sound with intense mental exhaustion following a challenging cognitive task. After repeated pairings, simply hearing the sound alone was enough to induce genuine fatigue – no further exertion required.
These examples illustrate how the brain’s 'alarm system' – designed to protect us – can become hypersensitive and remain activated long after any real danger has passed. Just as a fire alarm reacts as loudly to a waft of steam as to actual flames, the brain can generate very real symptoms even when the body is no longer under threat.
With the finishing touch of:Clarifying key studies
Two recent Norwegian investigations have been cited in ways that risk confusion:
1) The Loteca study tracked 382 young people after Covid-19 infection alongside 85 never-infected controls for six months. At follow-up, 49 per cent of the Covid group and 47 per cent of controls had symptoms that met WHO’s criteria for Long Covid – virtually the same proportion.
A narrower diagnosis of post-infectious fatigue syndrome (PIFS) was slightly more common in the Covid group (14 per cent vs. 8 per cent) but did not reach statistical significance. No biological marker reliably predicted who developed persistent symptoms; factors such as low baseline physical activity and loneliness stood out as important predictors.
2) The SIPCOV study enrolled 310 patients with Long Covid to compare a brief (2–8 hour) interdisciplinary intervention against standard care. Patients in the intervention arm were twice as likely to be classified as recovered – and these gains held steady at the one-year follow-up.
Moving forward, we urgently need open dialogue, informed by respect for patients’ experiences and a commitment to rigorous science.
We welcome dialogue with all who share this commitment.
Reme also has this comment on LOTECA and SIPCOV:
With the finishing touch of:
respect for patients’ experiences